Literal-Minded

Linguistic commentary from a guy who takes things too literally

Ordering Your Adjectives

Posted by Neal on July 14, 2011

It’s time to make good on my pledge to write about the three most interesting topic suggestions I got during the Grammar Girl book giveaway contest. Actually, I got significantly more than three suggestions that would be interesting to write about, and they’ll be a source of inspiration in months to come. But I only have three books to give away, so with some difficulty I’ve settled on the three topics that I’m going to write about first. (Well, first, second, and third, actually, but you get my generalized meaning of first, don’t you? Of course you do.) I’ll start with a suggestion from Carlos Iriarte, who wrote:

As a non native speaker, I would love to see a discussion about the possessive apostrophe, or the proper order of adjectives.

Carlos, you’ll have to go somewhere else to learn more about apostrophes, but as it happens, adjective ordering is a topic that I’ve been curious about for years. I remember trying to do a research paper on it as a grad student, for a seminar in lexical semantics, and finding the topic so complicated that I repeatedly narrowed the focus of the paper, eventually ended up with a sorry piece of work investigating restrictive and non-restrictive uses of just one single adjective, and got the paper back with the comment, “I would not spend any more time pursuing this line of research.” But a blog post, I think I can handle.


Cumulative (and pseudo-cumulative) adjectives

In many English grammars and usage guides, discussions of adjective ordering go hand-in-hand with advice on when series of adjectives should be punctuated with commas. These discussions usually talk about two kinds of adjective: cumulative and coordinate. With cumulative adjectives, you don’t use commas, but how do you know if you’re dealing with cumulative adjectives? There are two diagnostics, both of them based on the meaning of these adjectives, so I’ll start with the semantics of cumulative adjectives.

Suppose you have cumulative adjectives A1, A2, and A3, and noun N, in the string A1+A2+A3+N. A3 modifies N; A2 modifies the nominal A3+N; A1 modifies the nominal A2+[A3+N]. The best example of this kind of modification involves adjectives such as fake. For an example, we’ll use fake Italian marble. Italian modifies the noun marble, straightforwardly enough. Fake, however, doesn’t modify just marble. It builds on the adjective+noun cluster it’s sitting next to, and modifies the nominal Italian marble. Fake Italian marble could be fake marble from Italy, real marble from someplace other than Italy, or even fake marble from someplace other than Italy. If you want to zero in on the “fake marble” meaning, you’d say Italian fake marble. So in this example, the order of fake and Italian depends on the meaning you’re trying to convey. In other words, you can rearrange them, but you’ll have a different meaning.

This fact is the basis for the first diagnostic for cumulative adjectives: the “rearrangement test”. If you can’t rearrange the adjectives and have a grammatical phrase with the same meaning as the original, the adjectives are cumulative. The second diagnostic is the “and test”: Can you separate the adjectives with and and have a grammatical phrase that means the same thing as the original? If not, the adjectives are cumulative. Fake Italian marble fails the and test. Fake and Italian marble means marble that is definitely fake and definitely Italian. The meaning of real marble that’s not from Italy is no longer possible. So on both counts, fake and Italian are cumulative adjectives in the phrase fake Italian marble.

So far, so good, but with many other adjectives, we won’t get such a clear answer. There are many adjectives that have to go in a fairly fixed order, but for no apparent meaning-related reason. For example, how about little green bag? To a native speaker, ?green little bag sounds weird, so little and green clearly fail the rearrangement test.

This is where some grammar and usage guides go off the rails. They try to extend the reasoning for cases like fake Italian marble to cases like little green bag. They’ll say that little isn’t modifying just bag; it’s cumulatively modifying the chunk green bag. OK, let’s look at that a little closer. Let’s say bag refers to the set of bags. Green bag will refer to a subset of the set of bags, namely, the green ones. Little green bag will refer to a subset of the green bags, namely the ones that are also little. Perfect! Just the meaning we wanted! The trouble is that we can do the same exercise with ?green little bag and end up with the same set: bags that are both little and green. So we’re back to wondering why little has to come before green. The unfortunate students of English as a foreign language, meanwhile, are scratching their heads, trying to discern the big meaning difference between little green bag and green little bag.

Why did the rearrangement test work so well with fake Italian marble, but fail with little green bag? It has to do with the semantics of adjectives like fake and other adjectives. Adjectives like little and green are known as subsective adjectives, which means that, for example, green bag will refer to a subset of the set of bags. Fake, on the other hand, is a non-subsective adjective. (It’s also sometimes called a privative adjective.) In terms of our example, fake marble doesn’t refer to a kind of marble.

Anyway, let’s say that, having forced the same semantic reasoning on little green bag as we did with fake Italian marble, we’re now ready to run little green bag through the and test. Can we say little and green bag? To a native speaker, this will sound pretty awkward … or will it? To me it does, but so does five green and speckled frogs, which evidently sounded fine to the writer of that song. But if we ignore these misgivings, that would be a failure for the and test, which lets us conclude that little and green are cumulative adjectives here.

But even though we’ve managed to cram fake Italian and little green into the same cubbyhole, there’s a slightly altered and test that the grammar guides don’t use, which gives undeniably different results for fake Italian and little green. Instead of keeping the adjectives in attributive position (that is, modifying the noun), put them in predicative position (for example, after is/are) and then put in your ands. This marble is fake and Italian has the same problem as fake and Italian marble, so it fails both and tests. But The bag is little and green sounds fine — in fact, the and is mandatory — and we’re still talking about a bag that is both little and green. So little green bag fails one and test, but passes the other.

So here’s where we are so far: Non-subsective adjectives really do modify nouns (or adjective+noun clusters) in a cumulative way, and the name cumulative adjective fits. Their ordering is determined by the way their semantics work. More typical subsective adjectives don’t modify nouns in a cumulative way, and calling them cumulative adjectives is at best unhelpful and at worst downright confusing. So what determines the order you put them in, if not their meaning?

Linguists still do not have a definitive answer. What they do have is a hierarchy of adjective classes that (for whatever reason) occurs in a more-or-less fixed order in English. The order is fixed in other languages, too, though not all, and it’s not quite the same order across languages. Here is a composite hierarchy I’ve assembled from those given in several sources I’ve looked at:

evaluation size shape condition human propensity age color origin material attributive noun

I’ll call adjectives like these subsective fixed-order adjectives, but if you’re really used to thinking of them as cumulative adjectives, maybe the term pseudo-cumulative adjectives will go down better. The ordering hierarchy for these adjectives can be violated if there’s an adjective you want to put first for emphasis (I want the GREEN little bag, not the RED one!: COLOR>SIZE), or maybe to grab people’s attention (brown-eyed handsome man: COLOR>EVALUATION), or for unclear reasons (big bad wolf: SIZE>EVALUATION), but for the most part, following respecting the hierarchy will produce more native-like adjective orderings than choosing randomly.

Coordinate adjectives

Grammar and usage guides define coordinate adjectives as the opposite of cumulatives in every way. They’re separated by commas. They can be separated by and. They can be rearranged. They each modify the noun separately, not cumulatively. These four properties are valid; the question is, when do adjectives behave this way? The answer is that when you have more than one adjective in the same class from that hierarchy, they become coordinate adjectives. For example, there are four evaluative adjectives in the nominal phrase sexist, egotistical, lying, hypocritical bigot, from the 1980 movie 9 to 5. These adjectives could easily be rearranged…

egotistical, lying, hypocritical, sexist bigot
sexist, lying, egotistical, hypocritical bigot
lying, hypocritical, sexist, egotistical bigot
hypocritical, sexist, egotistical, lying, bigot

… but in the movie they never are. Multiple characters, including the boss himself, refer to the boss in this way, and part of the humor arises from the treatment of these coordinate adjectives as if they were arbitrary fixed-order adjectives, or as if sexist, egotistical, lying, hypocritical bigot were a common, recognized and lexicalized compound noun. The order was probably chosen in order to emphasize the rhyme between egotistical and hypocritical.

Here’s an example of mixed pseudo-cumlative and coordinate adjectives, from Weird Al Yankovic’s “Generic Blues” (from the same album as the song I wrote about here):

I’m just a no-good, scum-sucking, nose-picking, boot-licking, sniveling, groveling, worthless hunk of slime.
Nothing but a low-down, beer-bellied, bone-headed, pigeon-toed, turkey-necked, weasel-faced, worthless hunk of slime

These adjectives could be rearranged, although with some constraints. The negatively evaluative no-good and low-down probably need to stay where they are, at the beginnings of their respective strings of adjectives. In other words, they’re pseudo-cumulative with respect to the other adjectives. Worthless, if it goes anywhere else, would go with no-good and low-down; why Yankovic put it at the end I can only guess. Maybe for him, worthless hunk of slime was already a set phrase before he incorporated it into these lyrics. But the rest could be reordered among themselves; in other words, they are coordinate adjectives with respect to each other. Yankovic himself chose to cluster all the “human propensity” present-participial adjectives, and further separate them into direct-object-incorporating ones (scum-sucking, nose-picking, boot-licking) and intransitives (sniveling, groveling); and do the same with the “shape/physical appearance” past-participial adjectives involving body parts, with those involving animals further clustered. According to the hierarchy above, the human propensity adjectives would precede the physical appearance ones if they appeared in the same noun phrase, but that constraint could probably be pushed a bit.

The 9 to 5 and “Generic Blues” coordinate adjectives also pass the and test, although to get a result less subject to speaker variation, I’d go with the predicate-adjective version: The boss is sexist and egotistical and …, and This hunk of slime is low-down and beer-bellied and …. In attributive position, separating adjectives with and usually only sounds natural when they are (or sound like) they’re contradictory; for example, a complex legal and economic issue. Unfortunately, even the predicative version of the and test doesn’t work so well for adjectives like nose-picking and boot-licking, as speakers would probably say, “He picks his nose” or “He’s a boot-licker.” Oh, well, no test is perfect.

To sum up: If there’s definitely a meaning difference between different adjective orderings, let that determine how you order them, and don’t use commas. If you can’t find a meaning difference, don’t go trying to force there to be one. Instead, go by the adjective-ordering hierarchy, and don’t use commas. If more than one adjective has the same kind of meaning in the hierarchy, then use commas, or ands or buts if the adjectives have contrastive meanings.

Carlos, thanks for the suggestion, and I have a copy of Grammar Girl’s 101 Words Every High School Graduate Needs to Know to send you … as soon as you can give me a postal address.

29 Responses to “Ordering Your Adjectives”

  1. malkie said

    Is it possible that ‘the basis for the first diagnostic for cumulative adjectives [is] the “rearrangement test” ‘ may be begging the question a bit – especially for a non-native English speaker? Sometimes the problem is exactly that: determining whether rearranging the adjectives changes the meaning.

    • Neal said

      You bet it’s begging the question!

    • Neal said

      Oh, wait, you meant about the meaning difference for non-subsective adjectives. No, I don’t think so. I walked through the derivations of both fake Italian marble (Italian fake marble) and little green bag (green little bag). In the first case I ended up with different sets, but in the second I ended up with the same sets.

  2. wjvdr said

    I am completely in shock seeing the George Baker Selection mentioned on this blog… They’re not native speakers of English, so might just as easily have written “green little bag”. The adjective order in Dutch happens to be the same as in English, “kleine groene tas” (little green bag) sounds more natural than “groene kleine tas” (green little bag).

  3. Ran said

    Dr. Asya Pereltsvaig’s “Languages of the World” blog recently had a post, “Name your color”, that may help explain why it’s “little green bag” rather than ?”green little bag”. Or at least, it mentions various other phenomena that seem like they could be related in some way to that fact.

    In particular, she links to and elucidates some research that “suggests that children — and perhaps to some extent adults too — view colors as essential properties of typical objects” (emphases mine); for example, if I understand correctly, “tomatoes are red” because the typical tomato is red, even if plenty of individual tomatoes actually aren’t. This typical-essentialism leads young children to misidentify the color of an object if it belongs to a type of object that normally has a different color.

    • Ran said

      I retract the “emphases mine”. I *meant* to emphasize certain words, but it turns out that <u> doesn’t work here. Whoops!

  4. Gabe said

    Hoo boy, Neal, that was a great post! It’s not often that two of my favorite works of art as a child (9 to 5 and UHF) get mentioned in a single blog post. But more importantly, that was a really helpful analysis of adjective ordering, one of those things I’ve always been interested in but kept get scared off of by its complicatedness. You made them a lot less scary, although I’m going to take your advice and not try to wade into them for a paper any time soon.

    I did find a minor disagreement between our Englishes. I can move “no-good” and “low-down” around a bit more in “Generic Blues” than you can. I find them fine at the end of the list (perhaps inspired by Alexander and the Terrible, Horrible, No Good, Very Bad Day). I think I’m also okay with them in the middle as well; in that case, my only objection is that it’s breaking up a stream of human present-participial adjectives.

    • Neal said

      Thanks, Gabe!

      My intuitions are pretty soft here. One observation about the book, though: terrible, horrible, no good, and very bad all fall squarely in the set of “evaluative” adjectives, so whether no good comes near the beginning or the end of that list doesn’t say much; they’re all the same kind of adjectives. Of course, you don’t analyze it that much when you’re noticing that no good goes near the end of that list, so your point still stands.

  5. […] types of Australian accents; and Jamaican patois and the English schwa. Literal-Minded explored the ordering of adjectives, while K International mused on the translation of foreign store signage, teaching language with […]

  6. The Ridger said

    I once had an editor tell me I should capitalize “Old Navy” in this sentence “he was wearing an old navy blue sweatshirt”. I don’t believe any native speaker of English (which she was not) would have thought the brand name came before the color.

  7. Thank you Neal! Awesome article. I’ve sent my address to your facebook account in a private message 🙂

  8. Jonathon said

    Great write-up, Neal. I’ve always found it a little puzzling that grammar and usage guides don’t seem to make use of your second reordering rule (the predicative one), because it seems to me to be a pretty intuitive and obvious way of sorting things out.

  9. The categories in the composite hierarchy could do with some examples. Am I correct in thinking that “ugly big round worn-out annoying old grey Australian wooden toy” fills all the slots?

  10. The Ridger said

    @Jonathan: I think it’s so – not “intuitive” because it’s not universal, other languages do things otherwise* – but ingrained, that English speakers don’t have any trouble with it, hence nobody needs to be taught it. Few English speakers can come up with a listing like Neal’s, but offer ten of them a scrambled list (such as “Czech famous physics old frail two grey-haired little professors”) and they will almost always produce the same ordering. Guides don’t waste time on stuff nobody questions. I think ESL/EFL books do address the point.

    * For instance, in Russian “she lives in a big nice house” and “he looked at me with blue keen eyes” are neutral sentences, while in English they aren’t. This is troublesome to novice translators, who fear “changing” the original and thus are “faithful” but produce sentences fraught with implicatures that the originals didn’t have.

  11. ASG said

    Yes, yes, but you don’t answer the most important question of all… what was the adjective in your paper? 😉

    Seriously, though, this was a great post. Thanks.

  12. […] sequenza è stata compilata da Neil Whitman in Ordering Your Adjectives, un intervento abbastanza lungo ma che ho trovato molto interessante perché rende espliciti i […]

  13. […] Neil Whitman took a look at the literature on adjective ordering, and sums up thus: […]

  14. Most TEFL books have a similar table, and in fact this is a standard exercise for foreign learners at upper-intermediate level. TEFL books usually list pattern together with colour, but this could also be separated out, in which case it would come after colour and before origin etc – as in ‘an elegant blue polka-dot Italian silk tie’

  15. @Flesh-eating dragon – I think you missed the attributive noun, as in sports car, coffee cup or dinner jacket. But I would broaden the ‘attributive noun’ category to ‘purpose’, so as to include adjectives. Then you could have ‘an ugly big round worn-out annoying old grey Australian wooden educational toy’.

  16. If you are going for finest contents like I do, simply pay a quick visit this website every day because it offers quality
    contents, thanks

  17. Nawee said

    I’m glad I stumbled on your blog. It’s great to know that there are people who still love to think about languages. It’s disheartening sometimes when, as a non-native, I ask about the “little things” that are not really explained in the grammar book and I get told to just remember it or to stop looking for nuances. Thank for the article and your attempt at explaining this topic. I have a couple of questions though.

    1. What do you mean by “condition” and “human propensity”?

    2. Both height and weight come under the “size” category. Is there a particular reason why height comes first?

    • Neal said

      “Condition” and “human propensity” are just terms I copied over from the sources I read. I’m guessing condition could include adjectives such as “angry,” “unemployed,” and “healthy,” and human propensity could include more lasting properties, such as “cautious,” or “optimistic.” As for height and weight: “a short heavy man” vs. “a heavy short man.” Hmm. The first one definitely sounds better, but I don’t know why.

  18. […] looking for something slightly less professional — or at least, less academic — then in this blog posting, the writer posits an ordering […]

  19. […] This is the suggestion of language enthusiast Neal Whitman: […]

  20. […] This is the suggestion of language enthusiast Neal Whitman: […]

  21. […] This is the suggestion of language enthusiast Neal Whitman: […]

  22. […] Language Log’s Big bad modifier order | Mark Forsyth’s Bish bash bosh | Neil Whitman on Ordering Your Adjectives | A bit more technical but intriguing, Donka Minkova’s Ablaut reduplication in English: The […]

  23. […] question had run him straight into the old adjective-ordering issue. According to this table that I copied from my 2011 post on this […]

  24. Kate Schieber said

    Thank you for this treatment of the adjective ordering issue! I think the ablaut-reduplication IAO phenomena usually helps take care of any exceptions to the ordering rule. Can you speak to this? https://www.rd.com/culture/ablaut-reduplication/?fbclid=IwAR1dxs_Fb9F3aFgCEcpFdN3GbSPXbKz1godw8Qll9ea7Vdd-tkxUvYjxwh4

Leave a reply to Links: May 28, 2017 Cancel reply

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.